Research in Neuroscience

p-ISSN: 2326-1226    e-ISSN: 2326-1234

2017;  6(1): 1-4

doi:10.5923/j.neuroscience.20170601.01

 

Neurobehavioural Effects of Some Artemisinin-Combination Therapies in Prophylactic Murine Malaria Model

Koofreh G. Davies1, Innocent A. Edagha2

1Department of Physiology, Faculty of Basic Medical Sciences, University of Uyo, Nigeria

2Department of Anatomy, Faculty of Basic Medical Sciences, University of Uyo, Nigeria

Correspondence to: Koofreh G. Davies, Department of Physiology, Faculty of Basic Medical Sciences, University of Uyo, Nigeria.

Email:

Copyright © 2017 Scientific & Academic Publishing. All Rights Reserved.

This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution International License (CC BY).
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/

Abstract

Despite the widespread use of Artemisinin combination therapies in the treatment of malaria, there is no available study on the effect of these anti-malarials on neurobehaviour. This study was therefore designed to investigate effects ACTs on anxiety T-Maze and cognition. Twenty five female albino mice weighing 20-26 g were used for this study. Animals were acclimatized and randomly selected into five groups. Light and Dark Box was used to test for anxiety while cognition was tested for with T-maze. Result showed no derangement in any of the neurobehavioural parameters tested for. This result indicates that malaria infection may not affect anxiety and cognition in subjects who currently on prophylactic ACTs. Also, Dihydroartemisinin/Piperaquine, Artemether/Lumefantrine and Artesunate/Amodiaquine did not affect cognition and anxiety at clinical dosage.

Keywords: Artemisinin-Combination Therapy, Prophylaxis, Plasmodium berghei, Anxiety, Cognition

Cite this paper: Koofreh G. Davies, Innocent A. Edagha, Neurobehavioural Effects of Some Artemisinin-Combination Therapies in Prophylactic Murine Malaria Model, Research in Neuroscience , Vol. 6 No. 1, 2017, pp. 1-4. doi: 10.5923/j.neuroscience.20170601.01.

1. Background of the Study

Malaria is a mosquito-borne infectious disease affecting humans and other animals caused by parasitic protozoa belonging to the Plasmodium type. The mosquito bite introduces the parasite from the mosquito's saliva into a person’s blood (WHO, 2014). The symptoms of malaria typically include fever, fatigue, vomiting, and headaches. In severe cases it can cause yellow skin, seizures, coma, or death. Symptoms usually begin ten to fifteen days after being bitten. If not properly treated, people may have recurrences of the disease month’s later (Beare et al.., 2006). Five species of Plasmodium can infect and be spread by humans. Most deaths are caused by P. falciparum, because P. vivax, P. ovale, and P. malariae generally cause a milder form of malaria. The specie P. knowlesi rarely causes disease in humans (Caraballo, 2014; WHO, 2014). Malaria is typically diagnosed by the microscopic examination of blood using blood films (Sutherland and Hallent, 2009). The recommended treatment for malaria is a combination of antimalarial medications that includes an artemisinin. The second medication may be mefloquine, lumefantrine, or sulfadoxine/pyrimethamine. Quinine along with doxycycline may be used if an artemisinin is not available (WHO, 2010).
Artemisinin and its semi-synthetic derivatives are a group of drugs that possess the most rapid action of all current drugs against Plasmodium falciparum malaria (WHO, 2006). Artemisinins can be used alone, but this leads to a high rate of recrudescence (return of parasites) and other drugs are required to clear the body of all parasites and prevent recurrence. The WHO has recommended that artemisinin combination therapies (ACT) should be the first-line therapy for P. falciparum malaria worldwide (Douglas et al., 2010; WHO, 2006).
There were a lot of concerns regarding the use of artemisinins following reports that administration of high and prolonged doses of artemisinins in laboratory animals cause neurotoxicity (Nontprasert 2002). However, many other studies have also reported that the artemisinins are very safe when administered at clinical dose regimen (Meshnick, 2002). In some earlier studies, we also demonstrated that artesunate and artemether at clinical doses did not neurobehavioral parameters in laboratory animals (Davies et al., 2012; Davies and Akpan 2016). Generally, there is lack of data on neurological or neurobehavioral effects of artemisinin combination drugs. Studies examining neurobehavioral effects of anti-malarials are not common, as most studies tend to concentrate on the effects of these drugs on malaria parasite alone. Moreover, the few reports available are studies done on healthy animals. Our study appears to be the first to examine the effects of ACTs on malaria infected mice.
The aim of this study is to compare the cognitive and anxiety effects of ACTs in prophylactic malaria mice model.

2. Materials and Methods

Care for Animals
Twenty five female albino mice weighing 20-26 g were housed in groups of five in cages under standard laboratory conditions and fed with pellet feed. Good quality water was given and the beddings were changed regularly.
Preparation of Drugs
Artemisinin based combination therapy namely; Dihydroartemisinin/Piperaquine Artemether/Lumefantrine and Artesunate/Amodequine were purchased from a reputable Pharmacy in Uyo, Akwa Ibom State, Nigeria. The drugs were prepared as stated below
(1) 20mg tablet of Arthemeter/Lumefantrine was dissolved in 50ml of distilled water to produce a stock solution of 0.4mg/ml.
(2) 40mg tablet of Artesunate/Amodiaquine was dissolved in 40ml of distilled water to produce a stock solution of 1mg/ml.
(3) 100mg tablet of Dihydroartemisinin/Piperaquine was dissolved in 100ml of distilled water to produce a stock solution of 1mg/ml.
Acquisition of Donor Mice
Parasite Plasmodium berghei was obtained from the animal house of Basic Medical Science University of Uyo, Uyo. A standard inoculum of 1x107 of parasitized erythrocytes from a donor mouse in volumes of 0.4ml was used to infect the experimental animal intraperitoneally.
Table 1. Experimental Design
     
Animal Treatment Protocol
The research started by pre-treating the animals with their respective drugs for 3 days. Animals were infected with Plasmodium beighei beighei on the 4th day. Cognitive test was carried 3 days post infection (7th day). While anxiety test was performed a day after test for cognition (8th day).
Tests for Anxiety and Fear
Procedure:
The light-dark box (45 x 27 x 27 cm) is made of plywood and consists of two compartments of unequal size as described by Costall et al., (1989). The small compartment (18 x 27 cm) is painted black (light compartment) and the larger compartment (27 x 27 cm) is painted white (light compartment). These compartments are connected by a door (7.5 x 7.5 cm) located at floor level in the center of the wall between the two compartments. The floor is divided into 9 x 9 cm squares and is covered with Plexiglas. Both compartments are covered with lids of clear Plexiglas. A 60-Watt table lamp located 40-cm above the center of the white compartment provides bright illumination of white light. Mice are carried into the test room in their home cages and were handled by the base of their tails at all times. Mouse was placed at the centre of the light compartment facing the exit. After 5 minutes, it was removed from the box and returned to its home cage. The maze was then cleaned with methylated spirit and allowed to dry before another mouse was introduced.
Behaviours scored:
(i) Light chamber duration: length of time the animal spent in the light chamber.
(ii) Dark chamber duration: length of time the animal spent in the dark chamber.
(iii) Line crossing in light: Number of times the animal crossed a line drawn on the floor in the light chamber.
(iv) Line crossing in dark: Number of times the animal crossed a line drawn on the floor in the dark chamber.
Test for Cognition
Procedure
T-maze is a simple maze used in animal cognition experiments. It is shaped like the letter T, providing the rodent with a straight forward choice. T-mazes are used to study how the rodents function with memory and spatial learning through applying various stimuli. The T-maze is made up of the base, the left arm and the right arm. Habituation is involved, and then a reward is introduced. The reward can be feed, a scent or a novel object. Normally, the animal will spend more time in the arm where the new object is introduced. This experiment can also help in knowing the rodent’s preferences example could be the rodent’s food preference.
Mice were introduced at the base of the T-maze. Before introducing the animal, two objects of the same colour are placed one in the left arm and one in the right arm. The animal will spend almost equal time in both arms thus familiarizing itself with the object. This process is called habituation.
After 3 minutes, the animal is removed and one of the objects is removed and replaced with an object of different colour. Normally, the animal should spend more time with the new object.
Behaviours scored:
Left hand (A) duration: Time spent in the left hand side of the T-Maze.
Right hand (B) duration: Time spent in the Right hand side of the T-Maze (Robert et al., 2000; Muller et al., 1989; Shoji and Hagihara, 2012).

3. Results

Table 2. Neurobehavioural Parameters in Light and Dark Box
     
Table 3. Comparism of the Duration in the Right and Left Arm of the T-Maze
     

4. Discussion

This is about the first study to examine the effects of ACTs on mice infected with malaria parasite. Specifically, the study evaluated effects of the ACTs on cognition and anxiety. As stated above the ACTs are the current dugs recommended by the WHO for the treatment of both complicated and uncomplicated malaria. ACTs thus deserve more scrutiny because of their widespread usage. T-maze was used to evaluate spatial working memory while anxiety was assessed with light dark transition box. As shown in the result, the time spent in the reference arm of the T-maze was not significantly different between the control group and the experimental groups. The absence of memory deficit in the treated groups suggests that the ACTs used in this study namely, Dihydroartemisinin/Piperaquine Artemether/ Lumefantrine and Artesunate/Amodiaquine, did have any untoward effect on short-term memory. Our earlier studies, using Morris water maze, showed that daily, oral administration of 60mg/kgbwt of artemether for 28 days did not cause memory impairment (Davies et al., 2012). Similar observation was seen in artesunate at same dose level and duration (Davies and Akpan, 2016). Our finding, however, is not in support of an earlier report which showed that Artesunate/Amodiaquine caused impaired short-term memory (Onalapoolakunle et al., 2013). The reason for the difference between our report and the latter is not quite obvious.
The result also showed no memory impairment in the untreated mice. This finding is not surprising because on day 7 that the memory test was performed, the untreated mice did not show signs of cerebral malaria, as documented in the SHIRPA protocol (Rodgers, et al., 2001). However, what is not clear, is the reason the animals did not develop cerebral malaria by this time contrary to some reports that showed that cerebral malaria usually occur from 5 days post infection. In another study of ours (under review), Plasmodium beighei berghei infected albino mice showed signs of cerebral malaria by day 9 while mortality was noticed on day 11 but Desruisseaux et al. 2008 demonstrated impaired short term memory 7 days and 75% mortality by day 11 after infection in Plasmodium beighei ANKA infected C57BL/6 mice. The above authors used novel object recognition and neural systems underlying episodic memory to establish their findings. The reason for the difference between our study and the one cited above may be due to the fact that we worked on a different strain of mice. While the study employed C57BL/6, we used albino mice and there may be strain differences. Secondly, our study made use of Plasmodium beighei beighei whereas the study employed Plasmodium beighei ANKA. The latter strain is reported to be more virulent than the former. Finally, reduced virulence may result from continuous cycle of passage, of the parasite.
In this study the light and dark box was used to evaluate anxiety and fear. For the purpose of this research the parameters considered were the time duration spent in the light and dark chamber of the box, rearing, number of transition and line crossing. Except for rearing, there was no significant difference between the control and the experimental groups. The light/dark test is based on the innate aversion of rodents to brightly illuminated areas and on the spontaneous exploratory behaviour of rodents in response to novel environment and light (Crawley and Goodwin, 1980; Lister, 1987; Bourin and Hascoêt, 2003). The exploratory activity reflects the combined result of these tendencies in novel situations. Thus, in increase in behaviours in the white part of the light/dark test, indicates decrease anxiety. An increase in transitions without an increase in spontaneous locomotion is considered to reflect anxiolytic activity. The lack of significant difference in these parameters, between the control and the experimental groups, indicates that neither malaria infection nor ACTs affected anxiety in this study. Our observations appear to support report by Desruisseaux et al. 2008, which showed that malaria did not affect exploration of object and environment. Exploration not being affected is an indication that anxiety was not affected since change in anxiety status is usually accompanied change in exploration. As mentioned above rearing was the only parameter that was affected in the light/dark box test. Rearing was significantly lower in the groups that received artesunate/amodiaquine and dihydroartemisin/piperacin combinations. Decrease rearing denotes decrease exploration and increase anxiety. This observation, taken alone would tend to suggest that these drugs caused anxiety and thus reduce exploration. However, this finding alone is not sufficient to arrive at such conclusion. More so, a study by Onalapoolakunle et al. (2013) on the contrary, showed that artesunate/amodiaquine caused reduced anxiety and fear.

5. Conclusions

Findings in this study showed that indicates that malaria infection may not affect anxiety and cognition in subjects who currently on prophylactic ACTs. Also Arthemeter/Lumefantrine, Artesunate/Amodiaquine, and Dihydroartemisinin/Piperaquine cause did not affect anxiety and cognition at clinical doses.

References

[1]  Beare, N. A., Lewallen, S., Taylor, T. E., Molyneux, M. E. (2011). Redefining Cerebral Malaria by including Malaria Retinopathy. Future Microbiology, 6(3):349-55.
[2]  Beare, N. A., Taylor, T. E., Harding, S. P., Lewallen, S., Molyneux, M. E. (2006). Malaria Retinopathy: A newly Established Diagnostic Sign in Severe Malaria. American Journal of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene, 75(5):790-797.
[3]  Caraballo, H. (2014). Emergency Department Management of Mosquito-borne illness: Malaria, Dengue, and West Nile Virus. Emergency Medicine Practice, 26(1): 25-30.
[4]  Costall, B., Jones, B.J., Kelly, M.E., Naylor, R.J. and Tomkins, D.M. Exploration of Mice in a Black and White Test Box: Validation as a Model of Anxiety. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 1989; 32:777 ─785.
[5]  Crawley, J.N. and Goodwin, F.K. (1980). Preliminary report of a simple animal behaviour for the anxiolytic effects of benzodiazepines. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 13, 167–170.
[6]  Desruisseaux, M. S., Lacobas, D.A., Lacobas, S., Mukherjee, S., Weiss, L. M., Tanowitz, H. B. and Spray, D. C. (2008). Alterations in the Brain Transcriptome in Plasmodium Berghei ANKA Infected Mice. Journal of Neuroparasitology, 208(40): 1288-1290.
[7]  Davies K. G., Edagha, I. A., Peter, I. P., John, E. O. and Eme E. Osim (2012). Effects of Oral Artemether Suspension on Spatial Memory in Mice. European Journal of Scientific Research. 82(4):499-505.
[8]  Davies, K. G and Akpan, M. N. (2016). Effects of Oral Artesunate on Spatial Memory in Mice. Journal of International Research in Medical and Pharmaceutical Sciences, 9(4): 152-157, 2016.
[9]  Douglas, N. M., Anstey, N. M., Angus, B. J., Nosten, F. and Price, R. N. (2010). Artemisinin Combination Therapy for Vivax Malaria. Lancet Infectious Disease, 10(6):405-16.
[10]  Greenwood, B., Mutabingwa, T. (2002). "Malaria in 2002". Nature. 415 (6872): 670–2.
[11]  Hascoet, M., Bourin, M. and Dhonnchadha, B. (2001). The Mouse Light-Dark Paradigm: a review. Practical Neuropsychopharmacology, 25(3):141-166.
[12]  Lister RG. The Use of a Plus-Maze to Measure Anxiety in the Mouse. Psychopharmacology, 1987; 92: 180 ─185.
[13]  Meshnick, S. R. (2002). Artemisinin: Mechanisms of Toxicity. International Journal of parasitology, 32:1655 –1660.
[14]  Onaolapoolakunle, J., Onaolapo, A. Y., Aweemmanuel, O.1, Jibunor, N. and Oyeleke, B. (20130. Oral Artesunate-Amodiaquine Combination Causes Anxiolysis and Impaired Cognition in Healthy Swiss Mice. IOSR Journal of Pharmacy and Biological Sciences, 7(2): 97-102.
[15]  Rogers, D.C., Peters, J. And Martin, J. E. (2001). SHIRPA, a protocol for behavioral assessment: validation for longitudinal study of neurological dysfunction in mice. Neuroscience Letter, 306: 89-92.
[16]  Sutherland, C. J., Tanomsing, N., Nolder, D., Oguike, M., Jennison, C., Pukrittayakamee, S., Dolecek, G., Hien, T. T., do Rosario, V. E., Arez, A. P., Pinto, J., Michon, P., Escalante, A. A., Nosten, F. and Burke, M. (2010). Two nonrecombining Sympatric Forms of the Human Malaria Parasite Plasmodium Ovale occur Globally. Journal of Infectious Disease, 201(10): 1544-50.
[17]  Taylor, W. R., Hanson, J., Turner, G., White, N. J. and Dondorp, A. M. (2012). Respiratory Manifestations of Malaria. Chest, 142(2):492-505.
[18]  World Health Organization. Retrieved 13 February 2014.