[1] | Dexter, H.R., Cutler, B.L., & Moran, G. (1992). A test of voir dire as a remedy for the prejudicial effects of pretrial publicity. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22, 819-832. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1992.tb00926.x. |
[2] | Kerr, N.L., Niedermeier, K.E., & Kaplan, M.F. (1999). Bias in jurors vs. bias in juries: New evidence from the SDS perspective. Organizational behavior and Human decision processes, 80, 70-86. doi: 10.1006/obhd.1999.2855. |
[3] | Kramer, G. P., Kerr, N. L., & Carroll, J. S. (1990). Pretrial publicity, judicial remedies, and jury bias. Law and Human Behavior, 14, 409-438. doi: 10.1007/BF01044220. |
[4] | Otto, A.L., Penrod, S.D., & Dexter, H.R. (1994). The biasing impact of pretrial publicity on juror judgments. Law and Human Behavior, 18, 453-469. doi: 10.1007/BF01499050. |
[5] | Ruva, C. L., McEvoy, C., & Bryant, J. B. (2007). Effects of pretrial publicity and collaboration on juror bias and source monitoring errors. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21, 45-67. doi: 10.1002/acp.1254. |
[6] | Banks, M. (2005). Spaces of (in) security: Media and fear of crime in a local context. Crime, Media, Culture,1, 169-187. doi: 10.1177/1741659005054020. |
[7] | Peelo, M. (2006). Framing homicide narratives in newspapers: Mediated witness and the construction of virtual victimhood. Crime, Media, Culture, 2, 159-175. doi:10.1177/1741659006065404. |
[8] | Dixon, T. L. & Linz, D. (2002). Television news, prejudicial pretrial publicity, and the depiction of race. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 46, 112-136. doi: 10.1207/s15506878jobem4601_7. |
[9] | Steblay, N. M., Besirevic, J., Fulero, S. M., & Jimenez- Lorente, B. (1999). The effects of pretrial publicity on juror verdicts: A meta-analytic review. Law and Human Behavior, 23, 219-235. doi: 10.1023/A:1022325019080. |
[10] | Studebaker, C. A., & Penrod, S. D. (1997). Pretrial publicity the media, the law, and common sense. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 3, 428-460. doi:10.1037/1076-8971.3.2-3.428. |
[11] | Ruva, C. L. & McEvoy, C. (2008). Negative and positive pretrial publicity affect juror memory and decision making. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14, 226-235. doi: 10.1037/1076-898X.14.3.226. |
[12] | Ruva, C. L., Guenther, C., & Yarbrough, A. (2011). Positive and negative pretrial publicity: The roles of impression formation, emotion, and predecisional distortion. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 38, 511-534. doi:10.1177/0093854811400823. |
[13] | Kovera, M.B. (2002). The effects of pretrial publicity on juror decisions: An examination of moderators and mediating mechanisms. Law and Human Behavior, 26, 43-72. doi: 10.1023/A:1013829224920. |
[14] | Ruva, C. L., Mayes, J. L., Dickman, M. C., & McEvoy, C. (2012). Timing and type of pretrial publicity affect mock-jurors’ decisions and predecisional distortion. International Journal of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, 2, 108-119. doi: 10.5923/j.ijpbs.20120204.06. |
[15] | Anderson, N. H. (1971). Integration theory and attitude change. Psychological Review, 7, 172-206. |
[16] | Costabile, K. A., & Klein, S. B. (2005). Finishing strong: Recency effects in juror judgments. Basic & Applied Social Psychology, 27, 47-58. doi:10.1207/s15324834basp2701_5. |
[17] | Dahl, L. C., Brimacombe, C. A. E, & Lindsay, D.S. (2009). Investigating investigators: How presentation order influences participant-investigators’ interpretations of eyewitness identification and alibi evidence. Law and Human Behavior, 33, 368-380. doi:10.1007/s10979-008-9151-y. |
[18] | Furnham, A. (1986). The robustness of the recency effect: Studies using legal evidence. Journal of General Psychology, 113, 351-357.doi:10.1080/00221309.1986.9711045. |
[19] | Kerstholt, J. H. & Jackson, J. L. (1998). Judicial decision making: Order of evidence presentation and availability of background information. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 12, 445-454. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720. |
[20] | Carlson, K. A., & Russo, J. E. (2001). Biased interpretation of evidence by mock jurors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7, 91-103.doi:10.1037/1076-898X.7.2.91. |
[21] | Schum, D. A. (1993). Argument structuring and evidence evaluation. In R. Hastie (Ed.), Inside the juror: The psychology of juror decision making (pp. 175-191). New York: Cambridge University Press. |
[22] | Anderson, N. H. (1965). Primacy effects in personality impression formation using a generalized order effect paradigm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2(1), 1-9. doi: 10.1037/h0021966. |
[23] | Anderson, N. H. (1968). Application of a linear-serial model to a personality-impression task using serial presentation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10(4), 354-362. doi:10.1037/h0026816. |
[24] | Dreben, E. K., Fiske, S. T., & Hastie, R. (1979). The independence of evaluative and item information: Impression and recall order effects in behavior-based impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1758-1768. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.37.10.1758. |
[25] | Devine, P. G., & Ostrom, T. M. (1985). Cognitive mediation of inconsistency discounting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49(1), 5-21.doi:10.1037/0022-3514.49.1.5. |
[26] | Dempster, F. N. (1987). Time and the production of classroom learning: Discerning implications from basic research. Educational Psychologist, 22, 1-21. doi: 10.1207/s15326985ep2201_1. |
[27] | Pyc, M. A. & Dunlosky, J. (2010). Toward an understanding of students’ allocation of study time: Why do they decide to mass or space their practice? Memory and Cognition, 38, 431-440. doi:10.3758/MC.38.4.431. |
[28] | Pavlik, P. I., & Anderson, J. R. (2008). Using a model to compute the optimal schedule of practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14, 101-117. doi: 10.1037/1076-898X.14.2.101. |
[29] | Seabrook, R., Brown, G. D. A., & Solity, J. E. (2005). Distributed and massed practice: From laboratory to classroom. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 107-122. doi:10.1002/acp.1066. |
[30] | Kornell, N. (2009). Optimising learning using flashcards: Spacing is more effective than cramming. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 1297-1317. doi:10.1002/acp.1537. |
[31] | Johnson, M. K., Foley, M. A., Suengas, A.G., & Raye, C.L. (1988). Phenomenal characteristics of memories for perceived and imagined autobiographical events. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 117, 371-376. doi: 10.1037/0096- 3445.117.4.371. |
[32] | Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 3-28. doi: 10.1037/ 0033-2909.114.1.3. |
[33] | Lindsay, D. S. & Johnson, M. K. (1989). The eyewitness suggestibility effect and memory for source. Memory and Cognition, 17, 349-358. doi: 10.3758/BF03198473. |
[34] | Benjamin, A. S., & Craik, F. I. M. (2001). Parallel effects of aging and time pressure on memory for source: Evidence from the spacing effect. Memory & Cognition, 29, 691-697. doi:10.3758/BF03200471. |
[35] | Pritchard, M. E., & Keenan, J. M. (1999). Memory monitoring in mock jurors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 5, 152–168.doi:10.1037/1076-898X.5.2.152. |
[36] | Pritchard, M. E., & Keenan, J. M. (2002). Does jury deliberation really improve jurors’ memories? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16, 589-601. doi: 10.1002/acp.816. |
[37] | Ruva, C. L., & LeVasseur, M. (2012). Behind closed doors: The effect of pretrial publicity on jury deliberations. Psychology, Crime, & Law, 18, 431-452. doi: 10.1080/ 1068316X.2010.502120. |
[38] | Kalven, H., Jr., & Zeisel, H. (1966). The American jury. Boston: Little, Brown. |
[39] | Boyll, J. R. (1991). Psychological, cognitive, personality and interpersonal factors in jury verdicts. Law and Psychology Review, 15,163-184.Retrievedfrom:http://www.law.ua.edu/lawpsychology/ |
[40] | Insko, C. A. (1964). Primacy versus recency in persuasion as a function of the timing of arguments and measures. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 69(4), 381-391. doi: 10.1037/h0042765. |