[1] | M. I. Posner and Y. Cohen, “Components of visual orienting”, in: H. Bouma and G. G. Bouwhuis (Ed.), Attention and Performance, Hillsdale: Erlbaum, pp.531-556, 1984. |
[2] | J. Lupiáñez, B. Milliken, C. Solano, B. Weaver, and S. P. Tipper, “On the strategic modulation of the time course of facilitation and inhibition of return.”, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol.54, no.3, pp.753-73, 2001. |
[3] | F. Pestilli and M. Carrasco, “Attention enhances contrast sensitivity at cued and impairs it at uncued locations.”, Vision Research, vol.45, pp.1867-1875, 2005. |
[4] | F. Sengpiel and C. Blakemore, “Characteristics of surround inhibition in cat area 17.”, Experimental Brain Research, vol.116, pp.216-228, 1997. |
[5] | M. Ruz and J. Lupiàñez, “A review of attentional capture: On its automaticity and sensitivity to endogenous control”, Psicológica, vol.23, pp.283-310, 2001. |
[6] | S. A. F. Squella and L. E. Ribeiro-do-Valle, “Priming effects of a peripheral visual stimulus in simple and go/no-go tasks.”, Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research, vol.36, pp.247-261, 2003. |
[7] | D. D. Macea, G. A. Abbud, M. L. Lopes-de-Oliveira, N. B. Fuga, and L. E. Ribeiro-do-Valle, “Control of attention by a peripheral visual cue depends on whether the target is difficult to discriminate”, Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research, vol.39, no.7, pp.957-68, 2006. |
[8] | C. L. Folk and R. W. Remington, “Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: evidence for two forms of attentional capture”, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, vol.24, no.3, pp.847-58, 1998. |
[9] | C. L. Folk, R. W. Remington, and J. C. Johnston, “Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings”, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, vol.18, no.4, pp.1030 - 44, 1992. |
[10] | C. L. Folk, R. W. Remington, and J. H. Wright, “The structure of attentional control: contingent attentional capture by apparent motion, abrupt onset, and color.”, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, vol.20, no.2, pp.317-29, 1994. |
[11] | C. L. Folk and R. W. Remington, “Bottom-up priming of top-down attentional control settings.”, Vision Cognition, vol.16, no.2-3, pp.215-31, 2008. |
[12] | N. B. Fuga, “The influence of attention and forward masking on reaction time.”, M. Sci thesis, University of Sao Paulo, Brazil, 2002. |
[13] | V. A. Guedes, “Inhibitory effects of a visual cue in reaction time tasks.”, M. Sci. thesis, University of Sao Paulo, Brazil, 2007. |
[14] | D. Lamy, “Reevaluating the disengagement hypothesis.”, Acta Psychológica, vol.135, no.2, pp.127-9, 2010. |
[15] | A. R. Mayer, M. Seidenberg, J. M. Dorflinger, and S. M. Rao, “An event-related fMRI study of exogenous orienting: supporting evidence for the cortical basis of inhibition of return.”, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol.16, pp.1262 - 1271, 2004. |
[16] | D. J. Prime and L. M. Ward, “Cortical expressions of inhibition of return.”, Brain Research, vol.1072, pp.161-174, 2006. |
[17] | R. M. Klein, “Inhibition of Return.”, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol.4, no.4, pp.138-47, 2000. |
[18] | J. Pratt and M. Hirshhorn, “Examining the time course of facilitation and inhibition with simultaneous onset and offset cues”, Psychological Research, vol. 67, pp. 261-265, 2003. |
[19] | J. Theeuwes, “Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection.”, Acta Psychologica, vol.135, no.2, pp.77-99, 2010. |
[20] | B. G. Breitmeyer and Ögmen, “Visual masking: time slices through conscious and unconscious vision.”, Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2006. |
[21] | B. A. Castro-Barros, L. L. Righi, G. Grecchi, and L. E. Ribeiro do Valle, “Interlateral asymmetry in the time course of the effect of a peripheral prime stimulus”, Brain Cognition, vol.66, no.3, pp.256-79, 2008. |
[22] | S. L. Macknik and M. S. Livingstone, “Neuronal correlates of visibility and invisibility in the primate visual system.”, Nature Neuroscience, vol.1, no.2, pp.144-9, 1998. |
[23] | H. Ogmen, B. G. Breitmeyer, and R. Melvin, “The what and where in visual masking.”, Vision Research, vol. 43, pp.1337 - 1350, 2003. |
[24] | Y. Chen, S. Martinez-Conde, S. L. Macknik, Y. Bereshpolova, H. A. Swadlow, and J. M. Alonso, “Task difficulty modulates the activity of specific neuronal populations in primary visual cortex.”, Nature Neuroscience, vol.11, no.8, pp.974-82, 2008. |
[25] | M. Turatto, F. Benso, A. Facoetti, G. Galfano, G. G. Mascetti, and C. Umiltà, “Automatic and voluntary focusing of attention.”, Perception and Psychophysics, vol.62, pp.935 - 952, 2000. |
[26] | L. D. Zimba and H. C. Hughes, “Distractor-target interactions during directed visual attention.”, Spatial Vision, vol.2, pp.117-149, 1987. |
[27] | Y. Petrov, M. Carandini, and S. McKee, “Two distinct mechanisms of suppression in human vision.”, The Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 25, pp.8704-8707. |
[28] | H. Spitzer, R. Desimone, and J. Moran, “Increased attention enhances both behavioral and neuronal performance.”, Science, vol. 240, pp.338-340, 1988. |
[29] | C. E. Boudreau, T. H. Williford, and J. H. Maunsell, “Effects of task difficulty and target likelihood in area V4 of macaque monkeys”, Journal of Neurophysiology, vol.96, no. 5, pp.2377-87, 2006. |
[30] | Y. H. Kim, D. R. Gitelman, A. C. Nobre, T.B. Parrish, K. S. LaBar, and M. M. Mesulam, “The large-scale neural network for spatial attention displays multifunctional overlap but differential asymmetry.”, Neuroimage, vol.9, no.3, pp.269-77, 1999. |
[31] | J. M. Kincade, R. A. Abrams, S. V. Astafiev, G. L. Shulman, and M. Corbetta, “An event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study of voluntary and stimulus-driven orienting of attention.”, Journal of Neuroscience, vol.25, no.18, pp.4593-604, 2005. |