Journal of Health Science

p-ISSN: 2166-5966    e-ISSN: 2166-5990

2013;  3(1): 5-11

doi:10.5923/j.health.20130301.02

Validity and Reliability of the Social Skill Scale (SSS) as an Index of Social Competence for Preschool Children

Anme T.1, Shinohara R.2, Sugisawa Y.1, Tanaka E.1, Watanabe T.1, Hoshino T.3

1Faculty of Medicine, University of Tsukuba, Ibaragi, Japan

2Yamanash University, Yamanashi, Japan

3Universiy of Nagoya, Aichi, Japan

Correspondence to: Anme T., Faculty of Medicine, University of Tsukuba, Ibaragi, Japan.

Email:

Copyright © 2012 Scientific & Academic Publishing. All Rights Reserved.

Abstract

Thepurpose of this study was to validate the Social Skill Scale (SSS) as an evidence-based practical index of social competence. In total, 30,993 preschool children participated in the study. Childcare professionals evaluated participants with the SSS. Results indicated a highly rigid structure of 3 factors corresponding to social competence. Cronbach’s alpha coefficients ranged from 0.91 to 0.93 across the sampled age groups. These factors also positively correlated with theChild DevelopmentScale. The SSS is able to measure social competence with high validity and reliability. Thus, the SSS is a helpful tool for understanding the development of social competence.

Keywords: Social competence,Children, Evaluation, Scale

Cite this paper: Anme T., Shinohara R., Sugisawa Y., Tanaka E., Watanabe T., Hoshino T., Validity and Reliability of the Social Skill Scale (SSS) as an Index of Social Competence for Preschool Children, Journal of Health Science, Vol. 3 No. 1, 2013, pp. 5-11. doi: 10.5923/j.health.20130301.02.

1. Introduction

Social competence has been an area of research interest in the field of child development[1]. This growing interest stems from studies observing thateffective social skill development confers a wide rangeof interpersonal, academic, and mental health benefits for children[2][3]. Given thesebenefits, center-based social skill training has become an increasingly common component ofchildren’s preschoolexperience[4][5].
Social competence is determined by complex interactions between the individual, home and school environments, peer relationships, and the larger sociocultural context[6]. Given the increased number of children displaying impulsive behaviors and social maladjustment, societal preparedness for appropriate education and environments to foster pro-social behaviors among these children are needed.
Social competence is defined by how well an individual functions inrelation to other people, particularly with respect togetting along with others and forming close relationships[7].
From early dyadic relationships with caregivers, and play and social interactions with peers during thepreschool years, to the formation of peer networks and close friends, socialcompetence is viewed as a primary component ofhealthy functioning and development[8][9].
Social relationships established through interactions are a major source of security and a sense of belonging[10].
Social competence is also viewed as the ability to understand others in the context of a social interaction and engage in smooth communication with others. Thus, social competence should be evaluated by the interaction between a person and their social environment[11]. However, methodologies that consider people in conjunction with their social environment across developmental stages have not been well developed.
Assessments of early interactions have focused on measuring the quality of a child’s home environment and parenting, based on the theory that an early child-rearing environment greatly influences child development. Two instruments, namely the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME)[12] and the Index of Child Care Environment (ICCE)[13], are often used in research on child development.
The Interaction Rating Scale (IRS) was developed to measure a child’s social skills and a caregiver’s childrearing skills through observations of caregiver-child interactions, which is based on the HOME[12], SSRS (Social Skills Rating Systems)[14], and NCAST (Nursing Child Assessment Satellite Training) teaching scales[15]. The IRS contains 70 items for computing a behavioral score and 11 items for computing an impression score; these items are grouped into the following 10 subscales (the first 5 focus on a child’s social skills while the latter 5 assess a caregiver’s parenting skills): autonomy, responsiveness, empathy, motor regulation, emotional regulation, respect for autonomy development, respect for responsiveness development, respect for empathy development, respect for cognitive development, and respect for social-emotional development. This measure is used to evaluate, in less than 5 min, an observation appropriate for the assessment of interactions between caregivers and children from birth to 8 years of age. The reliability of the IRS is 0.91, and the IRS’s validity with the NCAST is, r =.89[16-18].
Even though these studies enhance the methods of evaluating social competence, it is useful to develop a scale via a questionnaire. We developed the Social Skill Scale for Preschool Children (SSS: see Appendix) as a questionnaire measure of social competence. The SSS is able to evaluate children’s social competence in a short period of time during daily situations.
Several studies assessing social competence have included common factors related to “empathy/cooperation,” “self-control,” and “assertion.” These 3 factors have been found to be stable between the ages of 1 and 6 among Japanese children in a longitudinal study[18].
It is essential to develop methods to evaluate social skills relevant to peer relationships among children in order to promote healthy development.No other study has done similar research as developing a practical index of social competence which is structurally valid over six age groups.
The purpose of this study was to clarify the constructive validity and reliability of the SSS as an evidence-based practical index of social competence.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

Participants were children enrolled in a longitudinal study of social development (“Japan Child CareCohort”) from 58 government authorized childcare centers all over Japan from 2000 to 2009. Data were obtained from 30,993 children (1-year-olds: 4,012, 2-year-olds: 5,758, 3-year-olds: 5,758, 4-year-olds: 5,762,5-year-olds: 5,682, and 6-year-olds: 4,021), and usable data were obtained from29,617 children(those whose social skills could be measured).
In order to comply with ethical standards prior to conducting the research, all parents signed informed consent forms and were made aware that they had the right to withdraw from the experiment at any time. A personal ID system was used to maintain confidentiality of personal information. Furthermore, all data were stored on a disk, which was password protected; only researchers who were granted permission had access to the data.
The ethics committee of the University of Tsukuba approved this study.

2.2. Measures

The SSS was used to assess children’s social competence. It was developed as a questionnaire version of the IRS[16-18], which evaluates child-caregiver interactions by observations available for children under the age of 8.
The SSS measures social competence through 24 questions. The evaluator completes the checklist composed of 24 items focusing on a child’s behavior in the class (e.g., “Cheers up and comforts a person who is missing someone”).
Each item within the subscales is assessed in a Yes or No (1 = Yes, 0 = No or not sure) format, and the sum of all items in the subscale provides the overall score.
A higher score indicates a higher level of social competence.
In previous studies, social competence from infancy to adolescence had been classified into 3 core dimensions[14, 19, and 20], which are cooperation, self-control, and assertion. The SSS consists of 3 subscales that measure cooperation (8 items), self-control (8 items), and assertion (8 items). Scores for all SSS subscales are derived from professionals who carefor children in childcare centers. We also referred to the Interaction Rating Scale between Children (IRSC)[21], which evaluates children’s peer interactions by observations available for children between 3- and 18-years-old.
The Child Development Scale[22], which is widely used among government authorized center-based care centers across Japan, was used to investigate relationships with the SSS.The Child Development Scale is constructed from 4 subscales: social competence, communication skills, vocabulary development, and intelligence development.

2.3. Analysis

Factor analysis usinga maximum-likelihoodoblique rotation methodwas conducted to assess the factor structure of the SSS over our 6 age groups. Statistical Analysis System software (SAS version 9.1) was used for data analysis.

3. Results

Table 1 showsstandardized regression coefficientsof the factor pattern matrix of the SSS. The 3dimensionsextracted were consistent with those of previous studies on social competence (i.e., cooperation, self-control, and assertion).
Table 2indicates that factor loadingsof the SSS were consistentacross the 1–6-year-old age range.
The Cronbach's alpha coefficients ranged from 0.91 to 0.93 across the 6 age groups (1-year-olds: 0.91, 2-year-olds: 0.93, 3-year-olds: 0.93, 4-year-olds: 0.93, 5-year-olds: 0.93, and 6-year-olds: 0.93).
Table 3and Figure1 show mean scores on the SSS for each age group. The scoreson each subscale increasedwith increasing age.
Table 4 shows significant, positive correlations between the SSS subscales (cooperation, self-control, and assertion) and the Child Development Scale (social competence, communication skills, vocabulary development, and intelligence development).

4. Discussion

Consistent with a main principle of developmental task theory[23], results of thisstudy demonstrate continuity within domains of social skills assessed by developmentally appropriate indices across time.Our analysis revealed a highly rigid structure of 3 factors, which were valid, universal components of social competence. However, it is importantto keep in mind that this finding is equally consistentwith the possibility that effective social competencewith peers is affected by general child development[24]. Nevertheless, our results suggest that the SSS is a reliable and valid measure of social competence in young children.
The social competence scale for child-caregiver interactions (IRS), and for adult interactions, referred to as the Interaction Rating Scale Advanced (IRSA), have previously been found to be reliable, valid, feasible, and practical tools for assessing social interactions over time[16-18, 21]. Three strengths of the SSS, which is derived from the IRS, are described below.
First, we used a large number of participants to clarify the stability of the SSS factors. This helps validate the ease of using this measure in practice; the SSS is highly adaptable because the subscale framework can be used across several age groups.
Second, the SSS can be used for comparative studies across international samples given that the measure is based on a universal framework of social competence. The subscales are based on several categories, which have been widely used in research assessing social competence indicators.
Third, we have evidence that the SSS has adequate construct and concurrent validity given the positive correlations observed between the SSS and the Child Development Scale.
However, limitations of this study should be noted.First, the SSS subscales might not cover all the dimensions of social competence, even though we used some of the most common frameworks of social competence to develop this scale. Second, while the SSS uses the same scoring standard as a standardized tool, different developmental features of certain items across developmental stages might need further assessment. Third, the present study focused on observed social skills.The social skill strategies that would beeffective for coping with others are not necessarily the same strategies that would best allowchildren to cope with themselves[25]. Future research should examine strategiesfor regulating social skills, such as anger andfear. Overall, our findings suggest that reframing an upsetting outcome,whether by reappraising its importance or outcome,is an effective approach for evaluating social skill development across childhood.
Table 1. Factor pattern matrix of SSS rotated to thee direct oblimin(standardied regression coefficient
     
Table 2. Factor analysis of SSS by age(Likelihood Method Oblimin)
     
Table 2. Factor analysis of SSS by age(Likelihood Method Oblimin)(continue)
     
Table 3. Means of SSS
     
Table 4. Significant correlations between SSS and Child Development Scale
     
Figure 1. Means of score by age

5. Conclusions

This studyprovides evidence of a simple measure that is able to validly and reliably assess children’s social competence. The SSS can be considered as an established, valid screening instrument that reflects attributes of social competence. This measure helps provide further evidence of the development of social competence, whichenhances our knowledge of the practicality of the SSS for practitioners and caregivers.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This research was supported by Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (23330174, 24653134).

Appendix: Social Skill Scale (SSS)

1. Cooperation: children’s cooperative and empathetic behaviors
(1) Cheers up and comforts a person who is missing someone
(2) Brings cheer to friends who look lonely
(3) Praises a friend's success
(4) Applauds a friend who has done something well
(5) Pleased at others’ successes
(6) Helps friends without encouragement from others
(7) Helps friends when friends get hurt
(8) Helps friends when asked
2. Self-Control: children’s behaviors that emerge in conflict situations
(1) Postpones gratification when requested
(2) Behaves well as required by the situation
(3) Waits to ask for something
(4) Does not interrupt another’s speech
(5) Borrows toys from others
(6) Shares toys or food with others
(7) Does not throw temper tantrums in public
(8) Waits for his/her turn
3. Assertion: children’s initiating behaviors
(1) Displays strong reactions when he/she is spoken to
(2) Evidences happiness when someone does something for him/her
(3) Makes eye contact when someone speaks to him/her
(4) Shows his/her feelings on his/her face
(5) Initiates talk with another person
(6) Makes eye contact when speaking with others
(7) Participates in a play group (company) when asked
(8) Expresses appropriate greetings to others

References

[1]  Campos J, Frankel C, Camras L, On the Nature of Emotion Regulation, Child Development, 75(2), 377–394, 2004.
[2]  EisenbergN, Spinrad TL, FabesRA, et al. The relations of effortful control and impulsivity to children’sresiliency and adjustment. Child Development, 75, 25–46, 2004.
[3]  Keane SP, Calkins SD. Predicting kindergarten peer social status from toddler and preschoolproblem behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,32, 409–423, 2004.
[4]  Domitrovich CE, Cortes R, Greenberg MT. Improving young children’s social and emotional competence: A randomized trial of the preschool “PATHS”curriculum. Journal of Primary Preventions, 28, 67–91, 2007.
[5]  Izard CE, Trentacosta CJ, King KA.Et al. An emotion-based prevention program forHead Start children. Early Education and Development,15, 407–422, 2004.
[6]  Riggio ER. Assessment of Basic Social Skills. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 51(3): 649–660. 1986.
[7]  Burt KB, Obradovic J, Long JD, et al. The Interplay of Social Competence and Psychopathology Over 20 Years:Testing Transactional and Cascade Models, Child Development, 79(2), 359–374, 2008
[8]  KimJ, Cicchetti D. A longitudinal study ofchild maltreatment, mother-child relationship qualityand maladjustment: The role of self-esteem and socialcompetence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32,341–354, 2004.
[9]  Parker JG, Rubin KH, Erath SA. Et al. Peer relationships, child development, and adjustment: A developmental psychopathologyperspective. In CicchettiD, Cohen DJ(Eds.), Developmental psychopathology, New York: Wiley, 419–493, 2006.
[10]  Rubin KH., Coplan R, Bowker J. Social withdrawal in childhood. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 141–171. 2009.
[11]  Feuerstein R, Rand Y, Hoffman MB. The dynamic assessment of retarded performers: The Learning Potential Assessment Device: Theory, instruments and techniques. Baltimore: University Park Press; 1979.
[12]  Caldwell BM, Bradley RH. Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment. Little Rock: Center for Research on Teaching and Learning, University of Arkansas, 1974.
[13]  Anme T, Segal U. Center-based evening child care: Implications for young children’s development. Early Childhood Education Journal, 30(3): 137–143. 2003.
[14]  Gresham FM, Elliott SN. Social Skills Rating System - Secondary. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. 1990.
[15]  Barnard K. Nursing Child Assessment Satellite Training Manual. 1994.
[16]  Anme T, Yato Y, Shinohara R, Sugisawa Y. The reliability and validity of the assessment method for children’s social competence: Parent-child Interaction Rating Scale. Japanese Journal of Human Science of Health-Social Services, 14, 23–31, 2007.
[17]  Anme T. Trajectories of social competence by using Interaction Rating Scale (IRS) as an evidence-based practical index of children’s social skills and parenting. Journal of Epidemiology, 20: 419–426. 2010.
[18]  Anme T. Gender differences of children’s social skills and parenting using Interaction Rating Scale (IRS). Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences, 2: 260–268, 2010.
[19]  Caldarella P, Merrell KW. Common dimensions of social skills of children and adolescents: taxonomy of positive behaviors. School Psychology Review, 26, 264–278. 1997.
[20]  Elksnin LK,Elksnin N. Teaching social skills to students with learning and behavior problems. Intervention in School and Clinic, 33(3), 131–141. 1998.
[21]  Anme T, Sugisawa Y, Shinohara R, et.al. Validity and Reliability of the Interaction Rating Scale between Children (IRSC) by Using Motion Capture Analysis of Head Movement. Public Health Research, in press
[22]  Anme T, Segal, UA. Child development and childcare in Japan, Journal of Early Childhood Research, 8(2), 193–210, 2010
[23]  Sroufe LA, Egeland B, Carlson EA. One socialworld: The integrated development of parent-child andpeer relationships. In Collins WA,LaursenB (Eds.),Relationships as developmental contexts (241–261).Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, 1999.
[24]  Masten AS, Roisman GI, Long JD, et al. Developmental cascades: Linking academic achievement and externalizing and internalizing symptoms over 20 years. Developmental Psychology, 41, 733–746, 2005.
[25]  Davis EL, Levine LJ, Lench HC, Quas JA. Metacognitive emotion regulation strategies:Children’s awareness that changing thoughts and goalscan alleviate negative emotions. Emotion, 10, 498–510, 2010.