Food and Public Health

p-ISSN: 2162-9412    e-ISSN: 2162-8440

2015;  5(5): 151-156

doi:10.5923/j.fph.20150505.01

Getting the Message: Framing Nutrition Labels to Increase Consumer Elaboration Likelihood

Gregory Clare1, Katie Burghardt2

1Design, Housing and Merchandising, Oklahoma State University, Stillwater, OK, USA

2Human Nutritional Sciences, Oklahoma State University, Stillwater, OK, USA

Correspondence to: Gregory Clare, Design, Housing and Merchandising, Oklahoma State University, Stillwater, OK, USA.

Email:

Copyright © 2015 Scientific & Academic Publishing. All Rights Reserved.

Abstract

An online experiment measured perceived differences between nutrition label modifications of 946 participants divided into three groups (n=316 Control, n=308 Calories, and n=322 Percentage). One group of subjects reviewed a FDA mandated label as a control, another group a calorie label supplement, and the third group a percentage label supplement. Across the label exposure groups, significant differences were found for need for cognition on evaluation of potential outcomes. Health consciousness demonstrated a direct positive significant relationship to evaluation and focus on positive outcomes factors of the evaluation of potential outcomes scale. Interaction effects were observed between need for cognition and health consciousness between treatment groups, suggesting potential benefits from nutrition label summary supplements.

Keywords: Consumer Behaviour, Labelling, Packaging

Cite this paper: Gregory Clare, Katie Burghardt, Getting the Message: Framing Nutrition Labels to Increase Consumer Elaboration Likelihood, Food and Public Health, Vol. 5 No. 5, 2015, pp. 151-156. doi: 10.5923/j.fph.20150505.01.

1. Introduction

Food labels provide standardized information to inform consumers about the nutritional components of different foods [1] Levy and Fein [2] proposed that nutrition labels offer consumers the potential to compare food products and make better decisions about the healthiness and nutritional value of their food choices. Calories, calories from fat, total fat, saturated fat, cholesterol, sodium, carbohydrate, dietary fiber, sugars, protein, vitamin A and C, calcium, and iron have to be present on the Nutrition facts label [3]. Despite a dramatic increase in the availability of nutrition information made available to consumers through many communication channels obesity and chronic diseases are on the rise [4]. This research study explores label supplements designed to highlight key information on nutrition labels and how this information informs consumer evaluation of food labels.
A study conducted by Post et al. [5] revealed that when an individual with a chronic disease is directed by a health professional to read the label the probability is only 50% that they will do so, and this fact suggests that improvements to nutrition label information and design may improve consumer responses to label information. Balasubramanian and Cole [6] demonstrated a linkage between motivation to act and nutritional knowledge of consumers suggesting that lower levels of nutrition knowledge and motivation are more likely to use label information to make healthy food choices.

2. Why do Consumers need to Understand the Nutrition Facts Panel?

It is important for consumers to understand the Nutrition Facts Panel so that they can betternavigate through nutrition label information versus marketing slogans and tactics intended to get them to purchase food products. Of the 49% of products found to contain some sort of nutrition marketing by Colby et al. [7], 48% of the products were actually high in sodium, sugar, and/or fat. Understanding and knowing how to use the Nutrition Facts Panel can lead to improved health, less chronic diseases and a longer life. Jasti & Kovacs [8] demonstrated the correlation of consumers that read nutrition labels consuming less trans-fat, a leading contributor to many chronic diseases.
People that use nutrition facts labels often have diets lower in fat and cholesterol, eat more fruits and vegetables, and have a higher level of nutrition understanding [9]. Therefore, a gap needs to be bridged between those who understand the information and those individuals who do not by understanding what consumer characteristics influence label use. Providing nutrition information in a simplified, comprehensive manner could increase the chance of consumers using nutrition facts as a tool, and therefore increase health and overall nutrition understanding.
Due to time constraints many consumers may not take the time to read and interpret label information. Drichoutis et al. [10] found that consumers with more time for shopping have greater tendencies to read nutrition labels. Some aspects of the current nutrition label may prevent consumers from effectively understanding the information presented andeye tracking studies have demonstrated label design factors such as placement, perceived relevance of information order, noise, and size of the label may enhance label use [11]. Research has shown between 45 and 80% of adults have reported using nutrition facts [12]. Label use may be more common for people buying a product for the first time and when evaluating alternatives [13].

3. Improving the Nutrition Facts Panel

The Elaboration Likelihood model [14] (ELM) is the theory chosen for this study to explore consumer understanding of nutrition labels and measure perceived differences between label designs. According to Barbara J. Wilson, “The ELM posits that variations in persuasive effects are a function of how people process information and the degree to which they engage in elaboration or issue-relevant thinking” [15]. This model has two groups into which individuals are categorized for their need for cognition. The first is the “central” route of thinking. This route requires a higher level of thinking and involvement, or high elaboration. Individuals are more likely to fall into this category when the topic being presented is relevant to them [15]. The second group includes those who think in a “peripheral” or low elaboration manner with less concern about the outcome of their food choices. These individuals tend to want things to be simple, or use some type of “cognitive shortcut” or heuristics to assess the information [15]. What the ELM tends to reveal is that when central processing occurs, individuals are more likely to maintain what they have learned over time, but if peripheral processing occurs, the habits or opinions of individuals are more likely to change in the future.
Research Hypothesis: Between subject samples (control, calorie label modification, percentage label modification) there are differences in at least one pair of means in need for cognition and health consciousness score across the three classes on elaboration of potential outcomes.
Null Hypothesis (H0): In the population from which the samples were drawn, there are no differences in mean elaboration of potential outcomes across the three subject samples (control, calories, percentage).
Cacioppo and Petty [14] created the need for cognition scale to assess if a person has a tendency to enjoy the process of thinking and be engaged in it. The researchers suspect that a person’s need for cognition may influence label reading behaviors directly and positively. An individual who demonstrates greater elaborative thinking and infers potential outcomes of decisions may correlate to their reported need for cognition in general. Therefore, we will test the relationship of need for cognition to evaluation of potential outcomes to better understand this relationship.
H1: Need for cognition demonstrates a direct positive relationship to evaluation of potential outcomes.
The Health Consciousness Scale (HCS) created by Gould [16] measures a participant’s involvement in his or her own health. Presumably, people with greater health consciousness scores indicating awareness of their health status will be more likely to interpret label information and evaluate how the specific food item(s) will impact their health more readily. Persons that report lower health consciousness scores may not demonstrate a direct positive relationship to evaluation of potential outcomes.
H2: Health consciousness demonstrates a direct positive relationship to evaluation of potential outcomes.
Another scale used was the Elaboration of Potential Outcomes Scale (the EPO scale), created by Nenkov, Inman and Hulland [17]. This scale is a:
“Generalized predisposition toward thinking about consequences, specifically, it captures the degree to which individuals: 1) generate potential consequences of their behaviors; 2) evaluate the likelihood and importance of these consequences; 3) encode anticipated end states, with a positive focus; 4) encode anticipated end states with a negative focus”, Bearden et al. [18].
The scale is useful to the study because nutrition choices are often made based on how an individual considers the possible outcomes of consuming a particular food item. An individual may consider what happens if they choose foods high in fat or sugar (obesity, diabetes, heart disease), or they may consider what occurs when healthful choices are made (weight loss/maintenance, strong bones, low cholesterol, decreased cancer risk, etc.). The researchers were also interested to learn if positive thinkers (determined by the “Positive Outcome Focus Dimension”) responded differently to our intervention than negative thinkers (determined by the “Negative Outcome Focus Dimension”).
The EPO scale is separated into three sub-scales based on factor loadings. The first six questions are considered the “Generation/Evaluation Dimension”. The second part is the “Positive Outcome Focus Dimension” and it consists of three questions. The last section is the “Negative Outcome Focus Dimension” and it contains four questions. The relationship of health consciousness scores is likely to vary in the component factors since the researchers believe that health consciousness is a positive behavior related to evaluation and the subject’s anticipation of favorable outcomes based on their attention to health details. On the other hand, low health consciousness may demonstrate a relationship to the subject’s tendency to expect that negative outcomes will or will not occur in an inverse relationship to subjects with a positive outcome focus and greater health consciousness.
We also measured the influence of participant levels of need for cognition on the component factors of the EPO scale to determine if the subject’s reported tendency to evaluate outcomes and focus on positive or negative is related to nutrition label modifications.
H3a: Health consciousness will demonstrate a direct positive relationship to the “evaluate” and “positive outcome” factors of the evaluation of potential outcomes scale.
H3b: Health consciousness will demonstrate an inverse relationship to the “negative outcome” focus factor.
H4a: Need for cognition will demonstrate a direct positive relationship to the “evaluate” and “positive outcome” factors.
H4b: Need for cognition will demonstrate an inverse relationship to the negative outcome focus factor.

4. Methods

Recent focus groups by the researchers revealed participants were concerned with calories, fat, sugars and protein in order of importance when reviewing nutrition labels, Clare & Burghardt [20]. Front of package labelling and novel label designs were of secondary importance to participants. Easy to understand labels with similar designs to the FDA label standards were preferred among focus group participants and justified an exploration of nutrition label variants using conventional FDA label design standards. The researchers felt it would be helpful to choose items without a large amount of servings in the container. Therefore, bottled beverages were an excellent choice, as smaller portions are easy to find, and a wide range of healthfulness was available. Two types of milk, two types of juice, and two types of bottled smoothie style drinks were chosen.
The survey data was collected using Qualtrics and an Amazon Mechanical Turk panel, 951 participants started and 946 individuals completed the experiment and survey. The survey consisted of informed consent documentation, exposure to a product and label, and a posttest. Participants of the online only survey group were compensated with a $1.00 payment for each completed survey.
Participants were exposed to one of three conditions. Each condition was chosen at random. The first condition was a control using the standard FDA label. The second condition consisted of the beverages with the nutrition facts label and a supplemental nutrition information panel with the caloric breakdown of the beverage, and the posttest. The second condition related to the ELM was believed to appeal to individuals of low elaboration, as it simplifies how one thinks about caloric breakdown of the drinks. The third condition contained the products with the nutrition facts label and a summary of macronutrient recommendations that shows what percent of each should be consumed on a daily basis based on age of the consumer (percentage label modification). The third condition relates to the ELM because the researchers believed it would likely appeal to individuals of high elaboration. Completing mental math to calculate the percentage of something consumed can take a considerable amount of time. Recommended macronutrient amounts were obtained from the Dietary Guidelines for Americans [21].
To determine if an individual was of high or low elaboration, we used the elaboration of potential outcome scale measured 1-4 (strongly agree to strongly disagree). To test the hypothesized model, a one-way ANOVA and multiple comparisons test was utilized. The three experimental groups (control, calories, and percentages) were assigned randomly. Each participant reviewed five identical nutrition label designs for five different beverages with varied nutritional content before completing the post-test survey. The survey measured the theoretical constructs of elaboration likelihood though the use of the elaboration of potential outcomes scale, the health consciousness scale, and the need for cognition scale.
The subject’s estimates of the elaboration likelihood for nutrition information stimuli were tested with a factorial ANOVA of three multi label groups (control, percentage, calories) by five (very low to very high) need for cognition ratings by five Health Consciousness (very low to very high). The three nutrition label group estimates were then compared to each of the three components of the elaboration of potential outcomes (evaluate, positive outcome, negative outcome) using a factorial ANOVA.

5. Findings

A total of 946 participants completed the online experiment and survey: Control n=316, Calories n=308 and Percentage n=322. 52.4% of the participants were male and 46.2% were women. Additional characteristics of the sample may be found in Table 1.
Table 1. Socio-demographic characteristics of the participants
     
The first group (Control) of participants reviewed standard FDA nutrition labels M=25.38, SD=4.70. The second group viewed the standard FDA label along with a simplified calorie chart that broke down where all of the calories came from in the beverages. M=26.20, SD=4.03. The third group was exposed to the standard FDA label with a supplementary chart that summarized daily recommendations of macronutrients for different age groups M=25.29, SD=4.79. This analysis produced a significant ANOVA, F (3.95, 2) = p<0.05, indicating that there were differences in these means. Therefore, the null hypothesis is rejected. Eta squared was .33, indicating a moderate effect size. Multiple comparisons with Tukey’s HSD test revealed that differences exist among pairwise comparisons of means with the mean for the calorie group being the highest, the mean for the control group in the middle, and the mean for the percentage group being the lowest. This study produced support for the idea that the best method of conveying nutrition label supplement information is the simplified calorie method for individuals who prefer to evaluate potential outcomes, have a greater need for cognition and higher health consciousness. Table 2 presents the factorial ANOVA source table.
Table 2. Significant dimensions of the need for cognition and health consciousness scales by treatment on elaboration of potential outcomes
     
The analysis was designed to assess the effects of treatment type, need for cognition, and health consciousness on the participants’ degree of elaboration likelihood using the EPO scale, and on the separate components of elaboration likelihood (evaluation, positive outcome focus, and negative outcome focus). The ANOVA revealed a significant interaction between treatment type and need for cognition F (8,883) = 2.38, p<0.05 and for treatment type and health consciousness F (8,883) = 2.41, p<0.05 on evaluation of potential outcomes. Therefore, hypothesis 1 is supported. When examining the different components of the EPO scale, the evaluate portion demonstrated no significant interaction effects between treatment and health consciousness or need for cognition. No significant relationships were found between treatment and need for cognition and health consciousness on negative outcomes. However, there was a significant relationship between treatment and need for cognition F (8,884) =2.12, p<0.05 and treatment and health consciousness F (8,884) = 3.71, p<0.05 for the positive outcome factor.
As predicted a main effect of need for cognition was observed for the combined evaluation of potential outcomes scale and for the evaluate and negative outcome focus scales from a one-way ANOVA. The relationship between need for cognition and positive outcome focus was not significant. Therefore, hypothesis 2 is not supported. The main effect of need for cognition on evaluation of potential outcomes yielded an F ratio of F (4,943) = 3.82, P<0.01. The main effect of need for cognition on evaluate yielded an F ratio of F (4,943) = 11.91, p<0.001, lending partial support for hypothesis 4a. The main effect of need for cognition on negative outcome focus was not significant; therefore hypothesis 4b is not supported.
A similar main effect was observed for subjects reported health consciousness on evaluation of potential outcomes, the elaborate factor and the positive outcome factor. The main effect of health consciousness on evaluation of potential outcomes was not significant. The main effect of health consciousness on the evaluate factor yielded an F ratio of F (4,944) = 4.78, p<0.001. The main effect of health consciousness on positive outcome focus was F (4,944) =3.71, p<0.01. Therefore, hypothesis 3a is supported. The relationship between health consciousness and negative outcome focus was not significant and hypothesis 3b is not supported.
Most central to the purpose of this study was the observation of a statistically significant interaction between the calories and percentage label modifications by need for cognition on evaluation of potential outcomes. Similarly, statistical significance was observed between calories and percentage label modifications and health consciousness on evaluation of potential outcomes. Tukey post hoc tests were used to determine differences among groups using the hypothesized dimensions. For the evaluation of potential outcomes combined scale, statistically significant differences were observed between the calorie and percentage label modifications. When the entire scale was considered, calorie label supplements were preferred to the percentage label. Similar differences were observed between the calorie and control conditions (Mean Difference -0.799 p<0.05) and percentage and calorie conditions (Mean Difference -0.908, p<0.01) for the evaluate factor. Tukey post hoc tests for the positive and negative outcome factors were not statistically significant.

6. Conclusions

On February 27, 2014, the Food and Drug Administration prescribed updates to the Nutrition Facts label on packaged food items. These future labels will reflect serving sizes more accurately to the amount of food consumers tend to consume, and will focus on key areas including the serving sizes and calories. Calorie information will be presented in a larger font than current label mandates, Eisenman [22]. Overall, the fact that these study findings lend credence to the recommendations proposed by the FDA suggests that label changes are needed, and could possibly have a positive impact on the nutritional status of Americans by simplifying key label information design to emphasize cognitive processing methods.
It appears behavioral makeup (negative thinker, positive thinker, and evaluator), degree of health consciousness and need for cognition of an individual could impact a person’s perception of labels. However, further research is needed to confirm this. Upon the completion of our analysis, we also observed that presenting nutrition label information by providing the percentage chart to individuals who prefer to evaluate potential outcomes, have a greater need for cognition and a higher health consciousness may be effective. More research is required to support this observation.
A study conducted by Barone [23] concluded that the percent daily values would be of greater use to individuals who tend to evaluate a product in relation to their everyday diet. This finding is in line with this as those with a higher health consciousness; a greater need for cognition and an increased tendency to evaluate potential outcomes were more perceptive to the percentage chart created to compliment the %DVs on the Nutrition Facts panel. These findings also support the findings of Viswanathan & Hastak [24], who found that while %DVs might be helpful to consumers alone; the information could be enhanced if summary information was also provided.
A study in which nutrition information was presented to participants in different formats (additional information panels, multiple columns, or verbal descriptors) revealed that participants did not perceive the additional information effectively because it was not similar to what the standard FDA label looked like. The study attempted to standardize label supplements by adopting a design consistent with standard FDA lalels [25]. Perhaps those with a higher need for cognition, high health consciousness, and those who evaluate potential outcomes responded well to the percentage chart because it elaborated on the %DV information already present on the Nutrition Facts panel combined with requirements by age of the consumer. It is also likely certain participants preferred the percentage chart because it followed the same format (font, font size, layout) as the standard FDA label.

7. Limitations and Further Research

The participant population was split fairly evenly between male and female. However, many of the participants were between twenty-five and thirty four (42.1%), with the second greatest amount of participants being between twenty and twenty-four (21.7%). In both studies, the middle aged/older U.S. population was not well represented. The majority of participants had completed some college or obtained a four-year degree. This means those who have obtained a high school diploma/GED, have completed less than high school, or have obtained a Master’s degree or higher were not well represented. Most participants in the quantitative study made below $39,999 per year and below $20,000 per year. Therefore, individuals in the U.S. population who make $40,000 and over were not represented well. Lastly, as the online survey was a paid survey, it is possible that response bias, or the act of participants responding to questions in a manner they believe will please the questioner, could have occurred.
The findings from this study offer potential for an ongoing research agenda for label design to influence desirable consumer behaviors related to nutrition choices. First, while it was found that the percentage chart appealed to high health conscious individuals who prefer to evaluate potential outcomes and have a greater need for cognition, further study is needed to learn what type of label would appeal to a wider range of people. Secondly, a large-scale study that more fully represents the entire U.S. population would allow us to see if the results relate to other groups of people.

References

[1]  U.S. Food and Drug Administration. Nutrition Labeling and Education Act (NLEA) Requirements (8/94-2/95).Available fromhttp://www.fda.gov/iceci/inspections/inspectionguides/ucm074948.htm (2014).
[2]  Levy AS, Fein SB. Consumers’ ability to perform tasks using nutrition labels. J Nutr Ed, 1998; 30: 210-217.
[3]  Drewnowski A., Moskowitz H., Reisner M, Krieger B. Testing consumer perception of nutrient content claims using conjoint analysis. PubHlth Nutr, 2010; 13: 688-694.
[4]  Berning JP, Chouinard HH, Manning KC, McCluskey JJ, & Sprott DE. Identifying consumer preferences for nutrition information on grocery store shelf labels.Food Pol, 2010; 35: 429-436.
[5]  Post RE, Mainous III, AG, Diaz VA, Matheson EM, Everett CJ. Use of the nutrition facts label in chronic disease management: results from the national health and nutrition examination survey. J Amer Diet Ass, 2010; 110: 628-632.
[6]  Balasubramanian SK, Cole C. Consumers’ search and use of nutrition information: The challenge and promise of the nutrition labeling act. J Mktg, 2002; 66: 112-127.
[7]  Colby SE, Johnson L, Scheett A, & Hoverson B. Nutrition marketing on food labels. J Nutr Ed Bhvr, 2010; 42: 92-98.
[8]  Jasti, S, Kovacs S. Use of trans fat information on food labels and itsdeterminants in a multiethnic college student population. J Nutr Ed Bhvr.2010; 42: 307-314.
[9]  Misra, R. Knowledge, attitudes, and label use among college students. J Amer Diet Ass.2007; 107: 2130-2134.
[10]  Drichoutis AC, Lazaridis, P, Nayga Jr., RM. Consumers’ use of nutrition labels: a review of research studies and issues. Acad Mrktg Sci Rev, 2006: 1-25.
[11]  Graham DJ, Orquin JL, Visschers VHM. Eye tracking and nutrition label use: A review of the literature and recommendations for label enhancement. Food Pol, 2012; 37: 378-382.
[12]  Ollberding, NJ, Wolf, RL, Contento I. Food label use and its relation to dietary intake among US adults. J Am Diet Ass. 2010; 110: 1233-1237.
[13]  Driskell, JA, Schake, MC, Detter, HA.Using nutrition labeling as a potential tool for changing eating habits of university dining hall patrons. J Am Diet Ass.2008; 108: 2071-2076.
[14]  Cacioppo JT., & Petty RE.The need for cognition.J Pers Soc Psy, 1982; 42: 116-131.
[15]  Wilson BJ. Designing media messages about health and nutrition: whatstrategies are most effective? J Nutr Ed Bhvr, 2007; 39: S13-S19.
[16]  Gould SJ. Consumer attitudes toward health and health care: A differential perspective. J Cons Affr, 1988; 22, 96-118.
[17]  Nenkov GY, Inman JJ, Hulland J. Considering the future: The conceptualization and measurement of elaboration on potential outcomes. .J Cons Rsrch, 2008; 35, 126-141.
[18]  Bearden WO., Netemeyer RG, Haws, KL. Handbook of MarketingScales: Multi-Item Measures for Marketing and Consumer Behavior Research 3rdEdition. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications Inc, 2011.
[19]  Slater MD, & Rounder D. How message evaluation and source attributes may influence credibility assessment and belief change. J Mass Comm Qtr, 1996; 73: 974-991.
[20]  Clare G, Burghardt K. Getting the message: Consumer perceptions of front of package labels, Management 4(5), 2014.
[21]  U.S. Department of Agriculture & U.S. Department of Health and Human Services [Internet]. Dietary Guidelines for Americans 2010, Page 15. Available from www.dietaryguidelines.gov,2010.
[22]  Eisenman T. FDA proposed updates to Nutrition Facts label on food packages. Availablefromhttp://www.fda.gov/NewsEvents/Newsroom/PressAnnouncements/ucm387418.htm, 2014.
[23]  Barone MJ, Rose R L, Manning, KC, & Miniard PW. Another look at theimpact of reference information on consumer impressions of nutrition information. J Pub Pol Mktg, 1996;15: 55-62.
[24]  Viswanathan M, & Hastak M. The role of summary information in facilitating consumers’ comprehension of nutrition information. J Pub Pol Mrktg, 21:305-318.
[25]  Levy AS, Fein SB, Schucker RE. Performance characteristics of seven nutrition label formats. J Pub Pol Mrktg,1996; 15: 1-15.